File size: 48,416 Bytes
dae990d |
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846 847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863 864 865 866 867 868 869 870 871 872 873 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 883 884 885 886 887 888 889 890 891 892 893 894 895 896 897 898 899 900 901 902 903 904 905 906 907 908 909 910 911 912 913 914 915 916 917 918 919 920 921 922 923 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 933 934 935 936 937 938 939 940 941 942 943 944 945 946 947 948 949 950 951 952 953 954 955 956 957 958 959 960 961 962 963 964 965 966 967 968 969 970 971 972 973 974 975 976 977 978 979 980 981 982 983 984 985 986 987 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 995 996 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 1003 1004 1005 1006 1007 1008 1009 1010 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1020 1021 1022 1023 1024 1025 1026 1027 1028 1029 1030 1031 1032 1033 1034 1035 1036 1037 1038 1039 1040 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047 1048 1049 1050 1051 1052 1053 1054 1055 1056 1057 1058 1059 1060 1061 1062 1063 1064 1065 1066 1067 1068 1069 1070 1071 1072 1073 1074 1075 1076 1077 1078 1079 1080 1081 1082 1083 1084 1085 1086 1087 1088 1089 1090 1091 1092 1093 1094 1095 1096 1097 1098 1099 1100 1101 1102 1103 1104 1105 1106 1107 1108 1109 1110 1111 1112 1113 1114 1115 1116 1117 1118 1119 1120 1121 1122 1123 1124 1125 1126 1127 1128 1129 1130 1131 1132 1133 1134 1135 1136 1137 1138 1139 1140 1141 1142 1143 1144 1145 1146 1147 1148 1149 1150 1151 1152 1153 1154 1155 1156 1157 1158 1159 1160 1161 1162 1163 1164 1165 1166 1167 1168 1169 1170 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1180 1181 1182 1183 1184 1185 1186 1187 1188 1189 1190 1191 1192 1193 1194 1195 1196 1197 1198 1199 1200 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1210 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1220 1221 1222 1223 1224 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1230 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 1240 1241 1242 1243 1244 1245 1246 1247 1248 1249 1250 1251 1252 1253 1254 1255 1256 1257 1258 1259 1260 1261 1262 1263 1264 1265 1266 1267 1268 1269 1270 1271 1272 1273 1274 1275 1276 1277 1278 1279 1280 1281 1282 1283 1284 1285 1286 1287 1288 1289 1290 1291 1292 1293 1294 1295 1296 1297 1298 1299 1300 1301 1302 1303 1304 1305 1306 1307 1308 1309 1310 1311 1312 1313 1314 1315 1316 1317 1318 1319 1320 1321 1322 1323 1324 1325 1326 1327 1328 1329 1330 1331 1332 1333 1334 1335 1336 1337 1338 1339 1340 1341 1342 1343 1344 1345 1346 1347 1348 1349 1350 1351 1352 1353 1354 1355 1356 1357 1358 1359 1360 1361 1362 1363 1364 1365 1366 1367 1368 1369 1370 1371 1372 1373 1374 1375 1376 1377 1378 1379 1380 1381 1382 1383 1384 1385 1386 1387 1388 1389 1390 1391 1392 1393 1394 1395 1396 1397 1398 1399 1400 1401 1402 1403 1404 1405 1406 1407 1408 1409 1410 1411 1412 1413 1414 1415 1416 1417 1418 1419 1420 1421 1422 1423 1424 1425 1426 1427 1428 1429 1430 1431 1432 1433 1434 1435 1436 1437 1438 1439 1440 1441 1442 1443 1444 1445 1446 1447 1448 1449 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 1485 1486 1487 1488 1489 1490 1491 1492 1493 1494 1495 1496 1497 1498 1499 1500 1501 1502 1503 1504 1505 1506 1507 1508 1509 1510 1511 1512 1513 1514 1515 1516 1517 1518 1519 1520 1521 1522 1523 1524 1525 1526 1527 1528 1529 1530 1531 1532 1533 1534 1535 1536 1537 1538 1539 1540 1541 1542 1543 1544 1545 1546 1547 1548 1549 1550 1551 1552 1553 1554 1555 1556 1557 1558 1559 1560 1561 1562 1563 1564 1565 1566 1567 1568 1569 1570 1571 1572 1573 1574 1575 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 1581 1582 1583 1584 1585 1586 1587 1588 1589 1590 1591 1592 1593 1594 1595 1596 1597 1598 1599 1600 1601 1602 1603 1604 1605 1606 1607 1608 1609 1610 1611 1612 1613 1614 1615 1616 1617 1618 1619 1620 1621 1622 1623 1624 1625 1626 1627 1628 1629 1630 1631 1632 1633 1634 1635 1636 1637 1638 1639 1640 1641 1642 1643 1644 1645 1646 1647 1648 1649 1650 1651 1652 1653 1654 1655 1656 1657 1658 1659 1660 1661 1662 1663 1664 1665 1666 1667 1668 1669 1670 1671 1672 1673 1674 1675 1676 1677 1678 1679 1680 1681 1682 1683 1684 1685 1686 1687 1688 1689 1690 1691 1692 1693 1694 1695 1696 1697 1698 1699 1700 1701 1702 1703 1704 1705 1706 1707 1708 1709 1710 1711 1712 1713 1714 1715 1716 1717 1718 1719 1720 1721 1722 1723 1724 1725 1726 1727 1728 1729 1730 1731 1732 1733 1734 1735 1736 1737 1738 1739 1740 1741 1742 1743 1744 1745 1746 1747 1748 1749 1750 1751 1752 1753 1754 1755 1756 1757 1758 1759 1760 1761 1762 1763 1764 1765 1766 1767 1768 1769 1770 1771 1772 1773 1774 1775 1776 1777 1778 1779 1780 1781 1782 1783 1784 1785 1786 1787 1788 1789 1790 1791 1792 1793 1794 1795 1796 1797 1798 1799 1800 1801 1802 1803 1804 1805 1806 1807 1808 1809 1810 1811 1812 1813 1814 1815 1816 1817 1818 1819 1820 1821 1822 1823 1824 1825 1826 1827 1828 1829 1830 1831 1832 1833 1834 1835 1836 1837 1838 1839 1840 1841 1842 1843 1844 1845 1846 1847 1848 1849 1850 1851 1852 1853 1854 1855 1856 1857 1858 1859 1860 1861 1862 1863 1864 1865 1866 1867 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874 1875 1876 1877 1878 1879 1880 1881 1882 1883 1884 1885 1886 1887 1888 1889 1890 1891 1892 1893 1894 1895 1896 1897 1898 1899 1900 1901 1902 1903 1904 1905 1906 1907 1908 1909 1910 1911 1912 1913 1914 1915 1916 1917 1918 1919 1920 1921 1922 1923 1924 1925 1926 1927 1928 1929 1930 1931 1932 1933 1934 1935 1936 1937 1938 1939 1940 1941 1942 1943 1944 1945 1946 1947 1948 1949 1950 1951 1952 1953 1954 1955 1956 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 1976 1977 1978 1979 1980 1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015 2016 2017 2018 2019 2020 2021 2022 2023 2024 2025 2026 2027 2028 2029 2030 2031 2032 2033 2034 2035 2036 2037 2038 2039 2040 2041 2042 2043 2044 2045 2046 2047 2048 2049 2050 2051 2052 2053 2054 2055 2056 2057 2058 2059 2060 2061 2062 2063 2064 2065 2066 2067 2068 2069 2070 2071 2072 2073 2074 2075 2076 2077 2078 2079 2080 2081 2082 2083 2084 2085 2086 2087 2088 2089 2090 2091 2092 2093 2094 2095 2096 2097 2098 2099 2100 2101 2102 2103 2104 2105 2106 2107 2108 2109 2110 2111 2112 2113 2114 2115 2116 2117 2118 2119 2120 2121 2122 2123 2124 2125 2126 2127 2128 2129 2130 2131 2132 2133 2134 2135 2136 2137 2138 2139 2140 2141 2142 2143 2144 2145 2146 2147 2148 2149 2150 2151 2152 2153 2154 2155 2156 2157 2158 2159 2160 2161 2162 2163 2164 2165 2166 2167 2168 2169 2170 2171 2172 2173 2174 2175 2176 2177 2178 2179 2180 2181 2182 2183 2184 2185 2186 2187 2188 2189 2190 2191 2192 2193 2194 2195 2196 2197 2198 2199 2200 2201 2202 2203 2204 2205 2206 2207 2208 2209 2210 2211 2212 2213 |
Okay. Is this working? Is this working. On the back? Okay. The task in this week's lectures is try and help you understand a little bit about what comes into the brain and what goes down. We're going to spend a lot of time in discussing what goes on between those two stages, but the access to the outside world and how we affect our muscles are the two primary functions of the brain. Yohannes Mueller was one of the parents of sensory science, along with Helmholtz in the late 19th century, and it's hard to put it better than this. So I'll just read this out from his book in 1935, where he detailed a lot of specific nerve energies. What Miller said was that the same cause, such as electricity, can simultaneously affect all sensory organs, since they are all sensitive to it, and yet every sensory nerve reacts to it differently. One never passes as light, another hears it a sound, another smells it, another tastes. The electricity, another one feels it as pain and shock. One nerve perceives a luminous feature through mechanical irritation. Another one hears it as buzzing. Another one senses it as pain. Sensation is not the conduction of a quality or state of external body to consciousness or the conduction of quality or state of our nerves to consciousness. Excited by an external cause. So in this lovely, flowery 19th century prose that we don't use, unfortunately now what we were trying to say is that we do not have access to the outside world. What we have access to in terms of our perceptions, our cognition is the activity of the sensory nerve fibres that sense the outside world and provide the signals to the rest of the brain. It seems rather commonplace now, but at the time it was quite a revolutionary idea. It has analogies in more modern sensory science, and when we would talk about things called labelled lines, where we think that individual nerve cells contribute to a particular quality or sensation, for example, one nerve cell might signal the readiness of something in the world, another one might signal the greenness of something. Another one might signal the fact that that object appeared in a particular part of your visual field or on a particular part of your skin. Those are the labels that are attached to the activity of those nerve cells. One of the major challenges of neuroscience is to understand how the activity of nerve cells is translated into perceptual and cognitive states. And we are not there yet. But what we do know a lot about is how it is that those nerve cells can provide the signals that we need to access information about the outside world. I can spend several lectures talking to you about the structure of sensory nerve cells and cells, but I'm not. And I'm going to try and instead try and communicate to you three general principles, which I think for me at least, are the basis for understanding sensation. The first is that sensory receptors and we will understand a bit more about sensory receptors in the moment and not evenly distributed. Different parts of the body have different densities of sensory receptors, and for that reason, we use different parts of our bodies for different things. For example, we touch stuff with our fingers. We have a high density of contraceptives. We look at things in particular ways. We trying to bring their gaze, our gaze on objects so that the objects are projected onto the centre of our visual field. There are more photoreceptors in the centre of our visual field, so these different densities of receptors have large implications for how the brain is structured. And I'll take you through that. The second principle is that sensory signals are sent to the cortex along parallel pathways. This doesn't have to be the case. We can imagine a sensory receptor trying to encode everything it can about the outside world and sending all those signals to the rest of the brain. Instead, it seems that some receptors encode something the readiness or the grain issues and others and other things the blueish ness or the green regions of the brain of the outside world. These signals are therefore sent along parallel pathways to the rest of the brain. And this parallelism is the idea that different parts of the outside world are represented within the same modality by different nerve cells is key to understanding how the signals get the cortex. And may also be key to understanding how the cortex is organised. And the third thing I really want to get through in this lecture is that cortex creates the cerebral cortex creates topographic maps of the sensory periphery. I hope you understand the sentence in detail by the time we get through this next 15 minutes. The idea is that the cortical represents representations that we have that we use to see, to feel, to hear. They are constructed representations of the outside world such that the map of the body or the visual field is projected onto the cerebral cortex. And these topographic maps are key to understanding how at least early parts of the cerebral cortex, the initial stages of perception are organised. So these are the three things I really want to try and get through to you in the next 50 minutes. As I said, I could spend five or six lectures on sensory receptors themselves. I'm not going to spend one slide. This is because the basic structure of sensory receptors is pretty similar, and there's only one thing you really need to know about it. That is that those sensory receptors take some form of what we want to say. Interruption of the sensory surface and convert that into a nerve nerve signal, a spiking action potential. For example, photons of light come through in the eye and hit the back of the retina where they sign photoreceptors. And those photoreceptors in turn, transduced that light into an electrochemical energy, which they then pass on to the rest of the brain touch receptors and specialised nerve endings which are sensitive to the displacement of the membrane. So when there's a pressure on to the skin that membrane displaces. And that in turn is converted into electrochemical energy and sent to the rest of the brain ordering auditory receptors. Hence those in the ear specialised receptors which in which those things sense the vibration of the membrane, the tympanic membrane, and they then transform that vibration into magical electrochemical energy that is sent to the rest of the brain. So these sensory nerve endings are all just simply basically transmuting that external stimulus into something that is an action potential. Effectively. I've learned a lot in that sentence, but it's what you need to think about. There's a couple of definitions I just want to get us through as well. The first is that essentially receptors signals the presence, that is the actual detects the presence of an object and signals at a location on the body. So if we can imagine a stimulus saying, let's just think the easiest to think of touching your skin, if you touch it very lightly, you can't feel it. Or you can because if someone else touches in very lightly, you would feel that is only when you make a strong enough indentation of the skin that you can feel pressure. And so as we discussed in a couple of lectures ago, now, those have thresholds. They need a minimum intensity of a stimulus to generate an action potential. And so you can imagine gradually increasing the amount of pressure that someone applies to your skin. And at some point, you will notice that. And that's because as the stimulus intensity increases, so there are small changes in the resting membrane potential of these nerve cells. And at some point in time, that small change is sufficient to drive the occurrence of an action potential. As we talked about in the third or fourth lecture. So there's a threshold, there's a minimum intensity which are below which you're not sensitive. INVISION Minimum intensity is several photons. In a perfectly black environment. We turn all life off here. We did what we call dark adaptation that is sat in the dark for half an hour. And your world photoreceptors incredibly sensitive. They can actually detect the presence of a few photons is really useful if you're running around the savanna late at night trying to avoid lions. And so they can be very sensitive, these receptors, but they still need a minimum level before they can sense something and signal something that would be, say, the threshold. The next point is that as you increase the intensity, the stimulus, at some point in time, you generate a number of action potentials. So at this point in time below here, below the stimulus intensity, this neurone may not be signalling the presence of the stimulus above that it does signal the presence of the stimulus and indeed the action potentials that the neurone produces increase with the intensity of the stimulus. So the number of action potentials is neurone produces signal something about the intensity of the stimulus in the outside world. This function is often called a sigmoid because it looks a bit like an S, and you'll be encountering it several times over the next few weeks. The second related concept is that all sensory neurones have receptive fields. While sometimes we try and work this out, but it's really, really simple. The idea is that sensory stimuli, even if it's an effective stimulus, let's just say it's a finger press on your arm. Each individual nerve cell is sensitive to a particular location on the arm that that finger is put. And if the finger is put somewhere else and the rest of the body at nerve cell is not going to sense it. So it has a receptive field. It has an area of the skin within which this object, your finger, needs to be placed for the pressure to elicit a response from that nerve so that its receptive field will go through that in the second. The same concept can be thought of in terms of vision. In that case, nerve cells in a visual pathway may only respond if a light stimulus is placed in a certain part of the visual field, say up here and to the right. It won't respond if in space over here or down here or up here is the location in the visual field or exactly equivalently the location on the retina where that image is projected, where those receptive fields are. So that's a receptive field location on your body, whether on your body sensory surface, whether it is in the eye, the cochlea, wherever, whereabouts. So it takes information from. It only takes you from a limited part of the visual world, from limb, part of the body, etc.. And you can think of this. We often think of these receptive fields as having a non-uniform sensitivity across that, and we'll get into that in a second. But the idea here is that basically at the periphery of the recipe field, the nerve cell is not very sensitive. You have to make very strong indentations for the nerve cell to respond, whereas in the centre of the receptive field, it's very sensitive in response to slightly weaker indentations. We can measure that by, for example, placing a stimulus at different points relative to the receptive field. And if we do that and measure the number of spikes that are produced by a neurone, we would see something like this profile, like a Gaussian, usually a normal distribution whereby the same stimulus is capable of finding many spikes when space in the centre of the receptive field, but only a few in space on the periphery. Now, the observant of you there will think, well, hang on a sec. If if there is any field means that the number of spikes in your produces for the same stimulus depends on the location with respect to this centre of the receptive field. And the number of spikes produced depends on the intensive stimulus. Isn't there a confound there? Isn't there something? Couldn't I trade off the intensity of stimulus for the spatial position and get the same number of action cancels from this nerve? So. And if you were thinking that that's precisely correct, that's one of the major compounds in sensory pathways, trying to extract the important things from what is often multiple different types of causes that could give rise to the same number of action potentials. And hopefully we'll be able to get through that a little bit later in this lecture. So I said that receptive fields at his low clients, discreet places in your skin or in your eyes or wherever with something is responsive to this is most nicely exhibited in the snout essentially system because the body the skin provides his surface, which he can explicitly think about or simply feels. And there's actually been some recordings from humans here where you put a small electrode into one of the nerves in the arm and you can, in five circumstances, pick up the activity of sensory nerve cells and those nerves. And you can map out the respective fields of both sensory nerve cells. And it turns out that they look a little bit like this, honey, to form circles on the hand here. And you see that they have different sizes such that they're a bit larger. If you're on the palm of the hand and it's quite a bit smaller from the fingertip. And you can think of that here. For example, having many small receptive fields on the thumb of the finger and large ones on the palm of the arm. Fewer of them. So it turns out this is, as I said, as a general principle, since we systems that distribution of receptors on the body or in the eye is not the same across the whole body. Some parts of the body have much higher density of receptors and some have much lower density. However, the whole surface is what we call tile or antisense receptors. That is, every part of the skin or every part of the eye has at least one sense for detecting stuff in there. So the combination of these two things, the timing and the sizes, has profound consequences on our sensory abilities. You can actually do this yourself, or rather you can do this with a partner. If you get a paper clip and expose the two ends, you can do what's called a two point discrimination. The idea there is that if you move the ends of the pits further and closer together, you can change the distance on the other parts of the skin that you're going to stimulate when you press that paperclip onto the skin. Now, if those two points are close enough together and you put it on, say your arm here, you detect, you experience the sensation of just having a single pinprick on your on your arm. If, on the other hand, you stuck that on the finger, you will detect or experience two different distinct pinpricks in your fingertip. So what's the reason for that? Why is it that the same stimulus feels like one thing? One's on your arm or on your palm for two things. One is on your fingertips. So the reason for that can be is logical from the structure of these receptive fields on your fingertips or on your thumb. The receptive fields are very small, so that seems distance apart of those two pinpricks will activate two distinct sensory receptors and evacuating two distinct sensory receptors, you will sense two distinct objects that are touching your skin. On the other hand, if those same interests are made on your arm, you're activating only a single sensory receptor. And if you activate only a single sensory receptor, you'll feel only one object on your skin. So the size of these receptors means that you can distinguish between two objects or one object being present. I'm not going to talk much about the subcortical pathways for presentation. The key point here in the Spanish sensory system, and as we'll discover in a second in the visual system, is that these signals are taken from the skin, in this case through the spinal cord, taken up through the thalamus, everything goes through the thalamus of cerebral cortex, and they project upon what is called the primary somatic sensory cortex. It's not a sensory to touch primary because that's the major source of input from the thalamus. So the primary cortices are those part of the cerebral cortex, the input from the thalamic relay cells for each different modality. And they form this, they project in this case onto the present. So we'll see in a second in the cerebral cortex. By the way, a lot of what we know about this is actually from work, from Walter Penfield and his colleagues in McGill. Back in the 1940s and fifties, where as they were preparing patients for surgery for epilepsy, actually made in different parts of the cerebral cortex. And that's what the patients felt. These experiments are no longer very often conducted, but they were incredibly illuminating at the time. They were able to map out in humans the structure of this amount of sensory cortex, for example, by asking people what they felt when you stimulated different parts of the cortex. So this is the central stool because these are going to be the potential supertankers get some fees. This is a central focus as part of the major Suvi in the brain and before pre and post after the simple superset is post mains behind towards the back of the brain premiums in front was a frontal brain and if you looked along this potential suitcase, you find a structure which is incredibly beautiful. What happens there is that if you stimulate different parts of this personal sulcus, some person will report that they feel sensations at different parts of their body. So, for example, if you down on the lateral side and you stimulate it, you might find that some reports that they felt a sensation on their face. Whereas if you're up on the top medial side, that report, instead of sensation on that front or on the foot or leg. So depending on whereabouts alone, this person was suicidal whereabouts and cerebral sucres are you are encoding. You are representing activity on different parts of your skin. I think this gives rise to the concept, which is the homunculus in terms of the touch among us means little man. And if you look at the representation of the body on this first episode, the touch representation, you find that this continuous representation of different parts of the body and different parts of the body that are closer together are generally speaking, represented closer together on the cortex. So if example, the foot is represented a similar location to the leg or the trunk, whereas if face the lips and the nose are represented close to each other and in between these of that hand in the arm. So you get this map of the body that's formed on this matter sensory cortex. It's the different types of touch that you can get in different parts of your body as represented on the similar sensory cortex. And you find that actually some parts of the body seem to be overrepresented. For example, the face is a large part of the sensory cortex, whereas the foot is quite a small part, even though its size is relatively speaking, even not in many cases in the face or the trunk, for example, occupies almost a minuscule part of the cerebral cortex. And the reason for that is fairly obvious if you think of just one simple principle. Every sensory receptor has about the same amount of cerebral cortex devoted to it. It follows then that we have more sensor receptors, for example, on your finger, on your thumb, on your face, you'll have more cerebral cortex devoted to that part of the body. And when you have less sensory receptors, it shrunk your arm, your leg, you have less part of the body devoted to that is what we would call cortical magnification. The idea that the cerebral cortex is like a magnifying glass onto your body. That magnifying glass, how much it magnifies depend on the density of the sensory receptors at that part of your body. This is what the locomotive looks like if you're trying to represent it as an intact human being. This cortical magnification has some implications. It means that your ability, as I said, to detect small changes in the position of objects or the presence of two objects instead of one depends on the density of sensory receptors and therefore the amount of cerebral cortex that's actually devoted to that part of the skin. This graph here shows you compares is perceptual, the psychophysical acuity that you have the different parts of your body and that low numbers in mean high acuity. That means you're very sensitive to small distances and large numbers being low acuity. That means you in much larger distances between objects to determine tunes at one there and can see that the areas of the body with the largest or the lowest acuity at a lower arm of the arm, shoulder, belly, back breast, thigh, half all those areas where you know yourself that when you touch those things, you're very less tense, very much less sensitive to the structure of the things that are touching that part of the body. Whereas for example, the fingers and the upper lip, the cheek, the nose have much higher acuity and much more sensitive to different the structure of the things that are touching the body. So this density of receptors determines the amount of cerebral cortex that is actually devoted to that part of the sensory apparatus. And that amount of cortex is devoted to that. Sensory in turn dictates how sensitive or how how much acuity you have, the sensory stimuli that impinge in that part of the body. I just briefly wanted to show you the structure, the visual pathways, very similar. I'm not going to go through all these lines. I just want to illustrate to you from a paper that we produced many years ago now, that in the eye there are retinal nerve cells which include photoreceptors that signal they're actually communicated by a little network of cells in the retina, which you learn about later stages via ganglion cells, whose axons make up the optic nerve. Those ganglion cells in turn go to this little structure in the thalamus the lateral clinic is, and from there their signals projected the primary visual cortex or V1. So very similar in structure to this, not a sensory pathway, except that one went through the spinal cord to get the thalamus and then the cortex or this one from the eye or straight through the optic nerve, through the thalamus and the visual cortex. There are different pathways from the eye to the thalamus, and that's to visual cortex. We often call these the P of the M or the PARTICELLE, and the Minnesota pathways is quite a bit in the reading that I suggested you do that discusses how the signals of these nerve cells differ. I just want to introduce you to the idea that they actually have different structure and different types of signals. Now, I said on the cement essentially surface, you can tell quite easily that the finger, for example, is what you used to touch on things, and you can feel the fine gradations in the texture, for example, the surface. Similarly, if you look in the eye, if you take a photo through the eye and this is what a photo through the eye looks like, you see an object. This is the bit where the optic nerve starts with the axons of the ganglion cells come out of the eye and go into the optic nerve. It's also the place where the blood vessels come into the eye from the optic nerve. This is the picture of the eye through a fan scope and in the middle of the eye. See, the structure is called the phobia. It's an incredible structure. And this structure, you've got no blood vessels. This is Photoshopped as a smaller than anywhere else in the body. And then the other apparatus in the retina has been pushed away. So the photos have to have direct access to the light that comes through the lens and hits the retina. And in this location, in this part of the eye, the small part of the eye is about three or four millimetres in size. You have this incredibly dense population of nerve cells called cone photoreceptors, and that's represented by this paper down here. You can see the density of these cones peaks in that area. And that means that there's because there's so many different, so many more photoreceptors in this particular part of the eye that you can distinguish between an object that's slightly displaced. So when I wanted to see the structure of the visual world, what I need to do is I need to move my eyes around so that part of the world that I'm interested in falls on the phobia. Because then I can distinguish the difference between what might be, for example, a happy face, a sad face, a bald face. So if I want to see that fine special detail, someone at the back of the room who's sitting there looking at me, that is about one quarter of my thumb. And is it slightly less than one degree of ice? If I want to be able to distinguish the difference between someone's eyes or their face to recognise their face, I have to bring my phobia onto that object. I have to move my eyes so that that part of the visual falls onto my phobia where I have this really dense array of photoreceptors. I won't talk about them at all. But the gods, the ones which are important for night vision actually more dense just outside of the phobia. And so it turns out if you're ever out there in the dark night looking at the stars, if you want to see a star, you don't look directly at it, you slightly side of it. And that's because of what photoreceptors are actually absent from the photograph from the centre of your gaze. And instead you need to bring that light onto the side of your phobia with regard to actually most dense. It turns out that this structure, this density photo receptors in the in the phobia, which is so much greater in the phobia than elsewhere in the eye, is paralleled by changes in the structure of the subsequent nerve cells in the eye. And the consequence is that we can't see fine spatial detail in the periphery of our vision. This again is the idea of cortical magnification. We can see fine spatial detail when we're looking directly at something, but not when away from that centre of our guys. Our cortex has magnified that small part of the visual field, which is occupied by the phobia. I think that's about the size of your thumb. That part of the visual field where all these thousands of kind photoreceptors are sitting waiting for light to come. The primary visual cortex is actually totally magnifying that part of the visual field. Some estimates would put it to be something like 20 or 30% of primary visual cortex is devoted to this tiny little part of the visual field, and the rest of the visual field is consequently represented by many fewer ganglion cells, cells in the cortex. So therefore we're much less capable of seeing the finer spatial detail away from the centre of gaze because the cortical magnification is so pronounced for us. For real. You can see this yourself. If you have a look at this slight demonstration here. If you look in the centre of this thing here on the projector, you should be able to read. If you look where the arrow is, you should be able to define or distinguish what each of the different letters is. Does everyone agree with that? Approximately. If you look at the centre, you can still see this on the left, the K on the right and left from bottom on the top. It's like going to. I then if, on the other hand, you look at the air over here, you should not be able to define many of the letters. They are present there. So you may be able to see that the ace in the S is there and you could see the K in the R, But many of the things like T.P. y are actually indecipherable to you. And the reason for that is that what I've done in making this diagram here is scale the size of the letters so that they occupy approximately the same amount of cerebral cortex when you're looking at the centre of the diagram. And because they'll find the same amount of cerebral cortex, you're equally able to see all those letters. But when we look at the eye and this is no longer matched, sometimes that is a much smaller and occupying enough of the cerebral cortex. The magnification is wrong. So this viewpoint. So you are less able to be able to detect what these different letters are. Hmm. I said that there were parallel pathways that take the signals from the eye to the visual cortex, and these are quite pronounced if you look in the thalamus. You can see in this little structure overlap, which is a beautiful structure. I spent most of my life studying it. So I think it's beautiful. It's called that unique because it looks a bit like a me. If you're younger than me, actually bend your knee, then you would look a little bit like me. And in this little genic nucleus, which you can see by the naked eye is these different layers or parts of magnet. So it layers smaller and larger cell bodies. And it is these layers. Now we know that these cells would communicate different things to the cerebral cortex. These different layers get different inputs from the eye, in particular these ganglion cells, the cells that form apple to the eye, to the thalamus. The particular ones are much smaller. They're going to pass through the magnet. So everyone's a much larger. They're going to be sterilised. And then these signals of these thalamic neurones then go to primary visual cortex. Now, turns out there's very few ways to test this. But the only in fact, the only way to really test is supply small lesions, as we discussed last week. You can place small lesions in the brain, in animals, in a small controlled lesions, and you can destroy some of the nerve cells in the palm. So all the magnet, some of the layers. And if you try an animal farm to report things about the outside world and therefore ask whether or not these nerve cells that come from the right things in the retina and send them to the cerebral cortex with these nerve cells saying different things, the cerebral cortex. And it turns out they do. So, for example, in this set of beautiful work is Bill Murray and to conduct in the late 1980s and 90. Summarised in this review that I cited here, the idea is that there's a lesion this place in the palm of the layers of the macaque monkey. Or in the magnesium layers of the of a different macaque monkey with monkey before the length made. This monkey is trained to report simple things about the outside world. What the colour they trying to simply report. What is something they will not. In a particular location. Hmm. If there was something that I went to make one important. If there wasn't a to make another report. These graphs here, the solid lines show the capacity of the animal to detect something which varies either in the kind of striping this of the patterns that are present or in the flicker. That is the kind of amount of times per second something flickers, a light flickers. Africa is defined by the tempo frequency. That is how many times a second something, because the spatial frequency here is just how many of these fine bars you have in one degree of visual angle, the solid limestone, what the monkey does in the normal case without a lesion and the different points. So the monkeys performance when you've after you've made a lesion, it turns out that if you make a lesion at the end pathway, you have almost no impact on the monkey's capacity to detect the spatial form of an object. Whereas if you make a leap into the pathway, this is almost most. On the other hand, if you make a lesion at the pathway you detect, you abolish the animal's capacities, the black lines here to detect very rapidly flickering things. Whereas if you abolish the pathway, we preserve the capacity to detect those rapidly flickering things. And finally, if you lesion the pathway, you kind of the monkey cannot see coloured objects, whereas if you lesion the pathway, the monkey can. So these things, these different perceptual abilities in the presence and absence of different pathways suggest that the different signals that come from the eye to the cortex carry qualitatively different and qualitatively different signals about the outside world that carried along the parallel pathways to the cerebral cortex where they are. Then we combine. So when we get to Cortex, we've got all these parallel pathways doing stuff in the sensory periphery, whether in visual cortex and other sensory cortex, wherever it is. We have these parallel pathways from a sensory final thalamus, bringing all this information up to the cortex. And somehow the cortex has to rearrange these interesting judgements about the outside world. For many years. Before that, the brain was basically you what you're born with. You had that there was no plasticity in the brain. It took several decades of experiments to actually reject that hypothesis. I want to show you a couple of those experiments in the substance lines on this line. These are some of the very early experiments that were able to reject the hypothesis that the brain, the cerebral cortex in particular, was indifferent to experience. It was the same when you were born as when you were dying. These two experiments relied again on work on monkeys because these monkeys were easy enough to be trained to report what had happened to to the outside world. In the first experiment here, I want to say is the monkey was trained to hold the fingers against a little rotating disc, and I had to make judgements about that rotating this, I think the direction of motion, that rotating disk using the fingers only. The question was, would the experience, the long term experience of making this judgement with your fingers change the representation of the fingers in the cerebral cortex? And the way that the researchers went about trying to address that question is that they made electrophysiological recordings from this ninth century cortex of these monkeys before, during and after training to do this simple task. And what they find is summarised in this slide here, which is a little bit complicated, but the end result is very straightforward. So this is recording from this mid-century area of an al monkey, people that monkey, and they're recording from the region that is most important in representing the hand associated before it's a monkey. Listen, the sensory cortex that's there now monkeys as it is in humans. What they did was they made recordings from these part of the cortex before experience, and they found a particular representation of the digits, the final four digits, five, which of the hand in this part of the cortex. And that's described over here. So is the fifth, fourth, second or third digit. And the normal here. In the normal case, these different digits have approximately equal parts of the cortex devoted to them. However, following this experience, following this training, you find a substantial overrepresentation of the second and the third digit, and those are the two digit, the two fingers that the animal is using to make this judgement. So prolonged exposure to these kind of tasks has changed how the cerebral cortex is organised. It has increased the amount of cerebral cortex that's important, that's used for extending information from those two digits. The animals used to do the task. So the brain is plastic. The organisation, the cerebral cortex is plastic. It can adapt to the structure of experience and tasks that we would need to accomplish. Although a substantial amount of work as shown this many different systems since, and this was the original work to show that this plasticity was there. I really encourage you to read it with a beautiful set of experiments. The converse is it can also be studied. That is what happens when you lose a digit or lose some part of your sensory periphery. In this case, if the animal, for example, loses a third digit. This case is surgically removed and the seizure. Again, you can make the recordings before and after that surgery. And again, you can measure in this case again from one case in the parts of the brain that represent the hand. And you find, at least in some cases and this is still controversial, that when the third digit is removed, a bit of the cerebral cortex that was responsible for including things that happened on that anger now include things that happened on the second or the fourth or the ninth and fingers. It's like this piece of cerebral cortex wants to do something, wants to do anything. And in the absence of any input from the third digit, it's asking for input from the second and the fourth digits help the brain represent things that are going on there. So the brain is the cerebral cortex is plastic. It can adapt to changes in the input from the outside world. Say this is not uncontroversial. In some systems, this seems to be less the case, and some systems seem to be more the case. It is certainly dependent on what kind of life injury happened early. Younger people who suffer injuries have more cortical plasticity. Older people have less. One of the things that this leads to is phantom limb. And I just want to spend a couple of seconds showing you a really effective video from one of the leaders in this field. Ramachandran. I find this quite an effective video, so just spend a couple minutes on. Twins First patients this Derek Steen. All right. One of Ramachandran first patients was Derek Steen. 13 years ago, he was involved in a motorcycle accident, and I pulled the nerves out of my spinal cord up in my neck. They told my parents directly that I would never use my arm again. About seven years ago, I was reading through the classifieds and I saw an ad in there. Amputees wanted that. It was a joke like that. It's just basically connecting the club to the ball. So I called the number and it was Dr. Ramachandran. Go relax. Today, Derek is teaching Ramachandran how to play golf. But several years ago, Derek made a crucial contribution to Ramachandran pioneering work in brain science. Yes, I was amazing. After my surgery, I sat up in the bed and still felt the arm there. Still felt everything there. And I'm looking down and I'm seeing nothing. It was pretty bizarre. The more I thought about it, the more it hurt. The more it hurt, the more I thought about it. So it was it was like it was never ending. I mean, I'd break out in a cold sweat and turn pale. Just standing here talking to you because the pain would hit so bad. If there is any one thing about our existence that we take for granted. It's the fact that we have a body. Each of us has a body. And, you know, you give it a name, it has a bank account and so on and so forth. But it turns out even your body is something that you construct in your mind. And this is what we call your body image. Now, of course, in my case, it's substantiated by the fact that I really use a body with bone and tissue. But the sense I have, the internal sense I have of the presence of a body and arms and all of that is, of course, constructed in my brain and it's in my mind. And the most striking evidence for this comes from these patients who have had an amputation and continue to feel the presence of the missing. How? It was the beginning of an important relationship. Important for Derek, because not only would he finally understand his phantom pain, he would also get to the bottom of a mysterious sensation he felt while shaving. When I first started shaving after my surgery, I would feel my absent hand start to hurt and tingle whenever I shaved this left side of my face. Meeting Derrick was important for Ramachandran because the explanation he came up with would rock the world of neuroscience. Photograph. That's just my arm. The first thing Ramachandran did was to invite Derek to his lab for a simple test that I want to touch different parts of your body. And I just want you to tell me what you feel and where you experience the sensation. Close your eyes. I could feel that on my forehead. Anything anywhere else? No. So my nose. Okay. My chest. Your chest. Okay. I can feel that on my cheek and I can feel rubbing on the phantom left hand. On the phantom left hand in addition to your cheek, I'm going to run the Q-Tip across your jaw and see what happens. I can feel like you did by my cheek and I can feel a stroking sensation across the phantom hand. You actually feel that stroking across your phantom hand. Okay, so that small visual video and you can just it goes on for a while. I encourage you to watch it. That shows the fact that this person has lost their arm, that some part of their representation of their body has distorted not just the inside of the brain, but also perceptually cognitively. And the likely explanation for this is that one part of the likely explanation for this is that the representation of the face is actually quite close to the representation of the hand. And as we saw with the monkey who is missing the third digit, when you lose inputs to certain parts of the cerebral cortex, that cortex seems to want to do something anyway, stop to draw input from neighbouring cortical areas. So that part of the body which was representing the arm is now no longer there. Now he's also drawing input from the face as clearly more complicated than just simply saying that because this person's body image is constructed, image is something that is not simply explained just by the amount of sensory cortex, but that distortion in the cortical representation is going to contribute to the fact that this person feels something, even though there is no longer there is plasticity is important in helping this, in helping the brain effectively try to reconstruct or to do what it would like to do, even in the absence of inputs. I just want to spend a couple of minutes facing what will be spending most of the next lecture on or the next one. And I'd really like you to do some reading in the next section, which is this review that I've put up online from Colby and go back to leaders in the field reviews to look at old. Now that is probably the best conceptualisation of the ideas we'll go through in the next lecture. We discussed that, that sometimes that is kind of a frame of reference in which you understand these sensations is is depends on how you want to think about things. I just want to explain to you what I mean by frames of reference for a few slides. So when we look at the cerebral cortex, we see this translated into many distinct areas and visual cortex, for example, as primary visual cortex. But then there's about three or four, maybe even ten or 15 different visual areas that sit next to primary visual cortex, the whole higher order cortical areas or association cortex. The same is the case in similar sensory cortex, the same effects in the auditory cortex. You have these primary areas, then you have these multiple other satellite areas. And the question arises, one that actually puzzled researchers for many decades now is why do you have so many cortical areas? Why don't we just have one area that's responsible for vision, one area that's responsible for autism? And the hypothesis that I'd like to explore in the next lecture is very much like these parallel pathways from the sensory periphery to cerebral cortex. These different cortical areas act as parallel representations or parallel constructions of the outside world. Each area is doing something, creating a slightly different interpretation of the outside world. This then raises the question of how can these different the things that are arising in these different cortical areas be brought back together? How can the different maps of the outside, both the construction of the outside world, be reconciled? And the second thing that starts when I ask is these topographical photographs and statements, these things that are maps of your body, your maps of your eyes, that's fine If we want to, you know, represent the precise location in our body that something happens. But it's not very useful if we want to move around the world where I need to know where my location is with respect to this table. With respect to the microphone. With respect to these chairs. So the question that arises is how these topographic maps of the sensory body of the body of the eye, how these transformed into something that could be behaviourally useful, could actually help us move around the world accomplish tasks. It was not very useful. Just simply know that this is a place of my hand or my arm. Sorry. I would like to know where that place is with respect. For example, if my arm is moved with respect to the rest of my body. So I want to order frame a frame of reference in which I can understand these different aspects of my movement throughout the world. And that's that is the majority past of what we call the parietal cortex. And that's what we're going to be spending the next half, the next lecture on. And as I said, I'd really like you to read that coding review because that will help you understand what it is that the cortex is trying to do, how it's constructing maps of the outside world that we can use to move around them. And so we, we, we investigated that on Friday, and I look forward to seeing you there. Thanks. Yeah. I was. Yes. So it's and particularly in two volumes. It was. Particularly a good topic for the show. But something about. Are you actually doing. It where it's kind of. Surprising for you to feel particular? I can't quite remember where where we are at with. I think the general sensation of like, I'm going to take on you, that's relatively easy enough to understand. This is the migration and it's kind of been associated with the reaction. You know how it is that. According to what's called a predictive coding framework in the world. Where you can predict quite well what the temptation is that you. Should get. Right because you're doing. Something and you can because you know what you should be getting ready to. Predict and therefore surprise what is predictable. And think there's a framework of understanding brain function, which Professor SEO has been particularly. Important from guidance. Which is that the job of the brain, you basically find out things that are not. And so a lot of the architecture, the brains that Christians are predicting, that includes, you know, perception, suppressing, not encoding things that, you know. I mean, for example, I think that. You can suppress your own sensations, especially during actions. So that's predictive protein, which is really influential in. On the questions regarding the object. Yes. So yes, in exactly the same way. It's easier to see the things overlap altogether. Like, yeah. That's where you can. Something. I think, optimism. And because here. And in my. You. Yeah. Yeah. The overlapping. For one thing, the idea of hiring people sometimes out. I'm very. Part of life, as you can imagine, is just a single. You would. But even in this case. Consequently. That often. That changes. Central brain function. Yes. Like many. And so even when you figure. It was final. Expect to really point and explain everything. Thank you. Thank you. So. And would the to. Yeah, I look at that because it's so much easier to like. Yeah. Okay. Yeah. You know, like in our. I mean, the idea is to. Always have an interactive story, but I don't know the exact exact. I like. I felt. I don't know. I mean. Yes. Okay. And I knew it would be like. Well, this. So people. And. We know. Know. I. You know this. Yeah. Yeah. Oh. Yeah. People. I think. And. |